TUTORIAL for the HANDBOOK FOR ACOUSTIC ECOLOGY


SPEECH ACOUSTICS

Interpersonal Dialogue



In any interpersonal dialogue, paralanguage, along with other non-verbal cues, creates, reflects and reinforces a relationship between the people involved. If they are meeting for the first time, there is an implied distance, but also perhaps cues to suggest a friendly narrowing of that distance, or at least to negotiate that option to see if the invitation will be accepted.

When a power imbalance is present in an interpersonal relationship, the paralanguage will likely reinforce that difference with a more formal type of interaction, a setting of boundaries, a lack of negotiation and maintenance of non-verbal differences. However, in a more progressive and open-ended situation, the one with the greater power, such as a teacher, may attempt to break down those differences by giving non-verbal cues that such formalities are not necessary, and co-operation, for instance, is possible.

With good friends, a lot of the interpersonal communication will likely be designed to reinforce the existing relationship, assuming it is satisfactory to both parties. The paralinguistic cues will likely be designed to match each other. In an interview situation, where you are meeting someone for the first time and want to elicit what is genuine for them to reveal, it is wise to match their paralinguistic cues, to allow their pauses, to react to what they have to say in a respectful manner.

Family situations are often the most complex, if not downright complicated situations for interpersonal dialogue, as all of the tendencies and strategies mentioned already can be involved at a non-verbal level. Moreover, everyone involved knows how to instantly interpret the paralinguistic cues and negotiate their response.

Here is an example of interpersonal negotiation between a son and his mother (joined at the end by the father), that occurred in under 25 seconds with four back-and-forth interactions where the paralinguistic cues and responses shifted in each interaction. It is a bit difficult to hear the first time round, because of background voices, but at SFU, we developed a technique of looping each interaction so that the paralinguistic contours could be easily identified.

This technique is similar to what psychologist Diana Deutsch termed “speech to song” over a decade later in the 1990s. By looping a phrase, its song-like qualities become readily apparent, which means that the paralanguage of pitch inflection, stress patterns and rhythms become more obvious, with less attention needing to be devoted to the verbal content.

We will call this a mini-opera in four acts, and present each interaction separately with four repetitions of the vocal sound, a rough outline of the pitch and rhythmic inflections as related to the text, and a detailed spectral representation of the recording.

Act One.


First interaction

Click to enlarge

In this initial interaction, the mother asks a relatively neutral question about where her son got the jacket he is wearing, putting the emphasis on “jacket” with a slight rise in pitch. After two beats of silence, the son mumbles something about getting it a few days ago. The paralanguage between the two couldn’t be more different. She speaks clearly in an upper middle register with normal loudness, while he speaks in a lower register, slurs his words and can barely be heard. She asks “where”, he replies “when”. Combined with the two beats of silence, his strategy clearly is: deflect the question. Maybe he guesses where this is leading.

Act Two.


Second interaction

Click to enlarge

In the second interaction, the mother acts surprised with an exclamation of “What” on a very high pitch, much louder than before, followed by the simple question “you bought it?”. The paralanguage now makes it clear what the issue is. The stress is not on “you bought it” (as in, you never buy your own clothes), nor is the stress on “you bought it” (meaning the jacket is not to her liking). No, the stress is on “you bought it” with the word “bought” landing on the beat. It’s doubtful she thought he might have borrowed or stolen it, so the issue is clearly the money involved.

The son this time waits only one beat, raises his pitch and loudness level, and improves his articulation somewhat to offer a defence that the jacket is intended for travel. The loop makes it clear that he is closing the gap between their vocal styles, as he says this in the same rhythm as the mother, putting the word “travel” on the same beat she has established. The strategy now is: offer a plausible defence.

Act Three.


  Third interaction

Click to enlarge

At this point, since neither is backing down, in the third interaction they decide to match each other’s paralanguage in a perfectly co-ordinated “song” with four beats. The key words fall precisely on the beat – “bought/jacket/travel” – with the final "yeah" on the syncopated half beat for emphasis, and no pause between their two phrases. Their pitch and loudness match each other as much as possible (she lowers hers, he raises his), and the articulation of the words is very clear. They are now in harmony, so to speak, sounding as equals.

Act Four.


  Fourth interaction

Click to enlarge

However, the harmony is short-lived, as the father joins the conversation and together with the mother they overwhelm their son, again emphasizing the issue of money. The son has no option but to retreat, and return to his initial low-pitched, low-level, slurred speech, tossing in a final “it wasn’t that much” as a final cadence. Here is the complete set of interactions, taking a mere 22 seconds.

Complete conversation

We won’t speculate further on the family dynamics (including why the parents didn’t know about the jacket until they were together at a party). What is remarkable – though not atypical in any way – is the speed of the paralanguage shifts and the economy of the actual text being used. This must be the result of their long-term experience of interacting as a family, complete with underlying tensions and evolving roles, all of which is reflected in the non-verbal type of behaviour we have experienced here.


Use the back button or return to the module index.